Maternal age and genetic variance in immunocompetence

22 October 2014

Evolutionary processes rely on the presence of additive genetic variance: evolutionary change is possible only if significant heritable variation in a phenotypic trait is present (Lynch and Walsh 1998). A substantial effort has been devoted to studying genetic variability and the interplay between genetic and environmental effects in shaping the evolution of quantitative traits (Ingleby et al. 2010; Nystrand et al. 2011; Wolinska and King 2009). Particular attention has been paid to genotype-by-environment interactions (GEIs), as they are regarded as a major force maintaining genetic variability in populations under natural selection (Lande and Shannon 1996; Roff 1997; Storfer 1996). However, the environment is not the only factor that may influence the expression of genetic variance. Sex- or age-specific expression of genetic variance may also contribute to our understanding of mechanisms maintaining genetic variability in traits undergoing selection (Charlesworth 2000; Hall et al. 2010; Seppala and Jokela 2010).

Introduction

Sex-specific additive genetic variance (VA) has been reported in several studies (Drobniak et al. 2010; Jensen et al. 2003; Poissant et al. 2010). It may be present in the form of sex-specific heritabilities (e.g. Drobniak et al. 2010; Jensen et al. 2003; Weiss et al. 2006) and as non-existing or even negative cross-sex genetic correlations (Drobniak et al. 2010; Poissant et al. 2010). In contrast, parental age has rarely been considered as a factor influencing the genetic variance. Such age-specific effects should be expected if specific genotypes survive across age classes, so different sets of alleles are transmitted by young and old parents. Age-related (within a specific individuals) changes in the breeding value have been demonstrated in a number of studies (e.g. Charmantier and Reale 2005; Wilson et al. 2007) (but see Brommer et al. 2010). However, it remains unclear whether parental age may affect genetic variance in the offspring.

Parental age constitutes an important determinant of offspring fitness (see Liu et al. 2011 for a recent review). Offspring of older parents reproduce at a lower rate (great tit Parus major; Bouwhuis et al. 2010) and show shorter life expectancy (fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster; Moore and Harris 2003; but see also Priest et al. 2002 – cockroach Nauphoeta cinerea). The mechanisms behind these age-specific effects are, however, poorly understood and clearly taxon-restricted. They are usually explained in terms of non-genetic age-specific parental effects (e.g. age-related reduction in the ability to provide sufficient parental care), but may also arise from genetic reasons (e.g. accumulation of mutations and age-related changes in genotypic interactions). Even if seemingly non-genetic, results of senescence may have significant quantitative genetic basis, which may profoundly alter evolutionary dynamics of traits and thus always should be considered in a quantitative genetic framework (Charmantier et al. 2014). To our knowledge only three studies attempted to study whether parental age influences age-specific genetic variance. An increase in genetic variance of morphological traits of the offspring with increasing parental age has been suggested in laboratory populations of the fruit fly (Drosophila melanogaster; Beardmore et al. 1975) and in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata; Beardmore and Shami 1985). In contrast, lower genetic variance in age at first reproduction was observed among offspring of older fathers in a wild population of blue-footed boobies (Sula nebouxii; Kim et al. 2011). Thus, genetic mechanisms may be responsible for possible age-specific decline in offspring performance. More studies focusing on natural populations are however needed, in particular because patterns of age-specific heritabilities may substantially differ between wild and laboratory populations with reduced selection (Beardmore and Shami 1985; Kim et al. 2011). Moreover, studying the influence of parental age on the genetic variance and evolutionary potential may open a new perspective in quantitative genetics, as such effects have usually been neglected in quantitative genetics analyses.

Here we experimentally test whether maternal age may affect additive genetic variance observed among offspring in the blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus). In our study, we estimate genetic variance in tarsus length, body weight and the immunological reaction to phytohaemagglutinin (PHA). These traits are often considered in quantitative genetics studies on birds and show moderate to high levels of additive genetic variance (Cichoń et al. 2006; Drobniak et al. 2010; Jensen et al. 2003; Kilpimaa et al. 2005; Merilä and Fry 1998; Pitala et al. 2009). These traits have also repeatedly been shown to influence reproductive success or survival and hence may constitute important selection targets (Alatalo and Lundberg 1986; Cichoń and Dubiec 2005; Garnett 1981; Møller and Saino 2004). In order to separate environmental and genetic variance we experimentally paired broods of females belonging to two distinct age classes and cross-fostered nestlings within these pairs. We analysed the resulting phenotypic data using an animal model (Henderson 1950; Henderson 1984; Kruuk and Hadfield 2007) which allows one to separate genetic and non-genetic sources of trait variance. We predict that additive genetic variance should differ between offspring mothered by young and old females. In contrast to the above-mentioned earlier studies we present rigorous analyses based on experimental age-based cross-fostering which provide a novel approach to studying age-specific genetic effects.

Materials and methods

Study system and field procedures

We studied a wild population of blue tits on the Baltic island of Gotland (57°01’N 18°16’E), about 120 km off the eastern Swedish coast (see Pärt and Gustafsson 1989 for a detailed description of the study area). The population of blue tits on Gotland is characterised by relatively high return and recruitment rates to the breeding grounds (40% and 16% respectively; own unpublished data), compared to continental populations. In this population, blue tits lay one clutch per season. Females lay on average 11 eggs (varying between 6 and 17 eggs). Young hatch after two weeks and fledge after the next 18-22 days. Individuals usually live up to three years, but individuals living five to seven years have also been recorded.

Our study was performed over three consecutive years (2004-2006). Each year, from the end of April, we inspected nest-boxes regularly to locate blue tit nests. For each nest the number of eggs, date of laying and date of hatching (day 0) were recorded. Nestlings were uniquely marked by nail clipping (day 2) and later by ringing (day 8). All nestlings were weighed at day 14th (electronic balance – Kern, Balingen, Germany, to the nearest 0.1 g) and measured for tarsus length (electronic calliper – Mitutoyo, Japan, to the nearest 0.01 mm).

To measure individual immune responsiveness to an unknown antigen we use delayed-type hypersensitivity reaction (Demas et al. 2011). To induce the reaction we injected phytohaemagglutinin (PHA) into the wing web of nestlings. PHA is a lectin derived from common bean seeds (Phaseolus vulgaris) that has a strong mitogenic effect on T lymphocytes (Goto et al. 1978). The hypersensitivity reaction involves cell-mediated, humoral and non-specific defense mechanisms (see Demas et al. 2011 for discussion), thus it may be considered as a general measure of readiness of immune system to fight antigens . 0.2 mg of PHA (Sigma Aldrich) suspended in 0.04 ml of saline was injected to the right wing web. The thickness of the wing web was measured thrice prior to and 24 hours (±1 hour) after the injection using a pressure-sensitive spess imeter (Mitutoyo, to the nearest 0.01 mm). All measurements were taken by the same person. The mean value of the three repeated measurements was used in further analyses. The level of hypersensitivity reaction was expressed as the intensity of swelling, i.e. the difference between the means of the first and post-24 hours measurements.

Both parents were caught when feeding young between day 11 and fledging. Unringed birds were fitted with a uniquely numbered leg-ring. Tarsus length and body mass of all captured breeders was recorded. Age (first-year, henceforth young females; or older, henceforth old females) was determined according to the presence of a distinct moult limit between greater and primary wing coverts in individuals born the previous year (one year old) or uniformly colored wing coverts in older individuals. Available age data indicate that majority of the older group were three years old individuals (approx. 60%), with a small proportion of 4-years old (~25%) and ≥5-years-old females (~15%). Of all females, only two were used twice in consecutive years; the remaining females are unique across all years.

In our study we matched newly hatched broods of young females and old females in quartets containing two young-mother nests and two old-mother nests. Nests within a quartet were matched by date of hatching (±1 day) and number of nestlings (±1 nestling). Two days after hatching we cross-fostered nestlings following a split-brood design, such that half of the nestlings were exchanged inside pairs containing a nest of the young female and a nest of the old female (Figure 1). The cross-fostering allowed us to separate additive genetic and common (post-hatching) environment effects (Kruuk and Hadfield 2007). Two randomly selected nests within a quartet (one nest of a young female and one of an old female) were subjected to brood-size manipulation (being enlarged by three nestlings coming from a nest not used in the quartets). Brood size manipulation was a subject of another study. However, as it is crossed with age-specific groups, the effect of brood manipulation should not be confounded with the effect of mother’s age (Figure 1). Thus, the brood size manipulation is included in our statistical analyses to account for a possible influence of brood enlargement, but we do not focus on this effect throughout the paper since in our system brood enlargement seems to have no effect on the genetic variance in the responsiveness to PHA (Drobniak et al. 2010).

In total, 25 quartets were created, evenly distributed across years. Our analyses comprise 1092 nestlings. In 2004 the hypersensitivity reaction was not measured and hence only 2005 and 2006 were considered in the analyses of PHA response. In total, 485 nestlings from 18 quartets were tested for the PHA response.

Quantitative genetic analyse

Data quality and preparation

We applied an animal model (a type of a linear mixed-effects model; Kruuk 2004) with age-dependent (co)variance structure to estimate genetic and environmental effects on PHA response, body mass on the 14th day and tarsus length. The models were fitted using ASReml-R 3.0 (Butler 2009) implemented in R (version 3.0.14; R Core Team 2014).

Animal model is a special case of a linear mixed model that uses all available genealogical information about relationship between individuals (i.e. a pedigree) to estimate the contribution of additive genetic effects to the total phenotypic variance (Henderson 1950; Henderson 1984; Kruuk 2004). Initially, our pedigree included 1317 individuals (offspring and their parents) from 3 cohorts (offspring from the years 2004, 2005 and 2006, plus their parents from generation preceding the year 2004). However, in the studied population, about 20% of offspring recruit in the following years and about 40% of adult individuals are observed more than once. Reduced recruitment is a common issue in open, wild populations that are not controlled with respect to the breeding design and therefore provide data with many missing links in the pedigree. Also, not all nestlings were measured for all analyzed traits. Thus, the effective number of individuals contributing to the estimation of additive genetic effects was reduced. After cleaning and pruning the pedigree using the pedantics R package (Morrissey and Wilson 2010), the number of individuals contributing to the estimation of genetic variance in body weight and tarsus length was 1090, with 874 maternities, 872 paternities, mean maternal sibship size of 7.8 and mean paternal sibship size of 8.2. For phytohaemagglutinin response 355 individuals contributed to the estimation of additive genetic variance, with 278 maternities, 278 paternities, mean maternal sibship size of 6.9 and mean paternal sibship size of 6.95. Our analyses are based on nestling phenotypes and since all nestlings were part of a large-scale cross-fostering procedure brood effects are not confounded with genetic effects in our analyses.

Animal models

Study year (2004, 2005, 2006), maternal age (young vs. old), and brood size manipulation (enlarged vs. control) were defined as fixed explanatory variables. To test for possible confounding influence of the interaction between maternal age and experimental treatment it was included in all initial models, but it appeared non-significant in all analyses (P > 0.5 in all cases), thus we do not consider this interaction in the presented results. Additional fixed effects were included in specific models. For body weight and tarsus length we included sex, to take into account a well-documented size dimorphism in the studied species (Blondel et al. 2002). The analysis of body weight included also tarsus length as a covariate, to correct weight measurements for the structural body size. Finally, in the analysis of PHA response we included body mass as a covariate, which is a usual practice accounting for the correlation between the body weight and the PHA-related skin swelling (Alonso-Alvarez and Tella 2001).

Following fixed effects, we modeled a number of random effects in all animal models: additive genetic effect (VA), nest-of-origin (termed origin henceforth), nest-of-rearing (termed rearing henceforth) and quartet identity. Interpretation of the non-genetic random effects is as follows: (i) origin estimates common origin variance, especially early maternal and common-environment effects (Lynch and Walsh 1998); (ii) rearing effect explains how much of the total variance comes from a shared rearing environment; (iii) quartet identity accounts for possible variance between quartets, emerging primarily due to differing hatching dates and other environment-related sources. To enable maternal age-specific genetic effects, additive genetic effects were modeled in the form of a 2×2 square covariance matrix, with two age-specific variances on its diagonal. Cross-fostering decouples brood (common environment) and genetic effects and thus in our analysis it was possible to estimate genetic covariance between two maternal age groups.

Testing of fixed and random effects

Fixed effects were tested using an adjusted Wald statistics (Butler 2009). Since random effects in our study system have implicitly hierarchical structure, significance of all random effects and age-related differences in genetic and residual variances were tested using likelihood-ratio test (LRT), using a sequence of models of increasing complexity (Pinheiro and Bates 2000). Likelihood-ratios for testing variances were assumed to follow a chi-squared distribution with df = 1, as always only one parameter more was estimated in the more complex model. (Self and Liang 1987) recommend a modified mixture chi-squared distribution (a mixture of χ2 with df = 1 and df = 0) for testing variances (for which the null-hypotheses are at the boundary of parameter – however, using chi-squared distribution with df = 1 is more conservative.

The most important part of the random effects structure – the maternal age-dependent genetic (co)variances – was tested by fitting a series of complex models. We predict that – under the null hypothesis – variances in two maternal age classes are equal and genetic correlation between these classes is equal to one. Verification of these hypotheses required the following models (we provide also the number of parameters describing the random effects part of the model, including all estimated random effects): (i) model assuming no differences in VA related to maternal age (five parameters estimated); (ii) model with maternal age-dependent VA (VA1 ≠ VA2) and genetic correlation between maternal age classes fixed at unity (rxage = 1; six parameters estimated); (iii) model with VA1 ≠ VA2 and unconstrained covariances (rxage ≤ 1; seven parameters estimated); (iv) model with VA1 ≠ VA2 and rxage = 1, but with residual variances differing between maternal age classes (to account for the possibility that heterogeneous residual variances might generate heterogeneous VA; seven parameters estimated). Models were compared in the order specified in Table 1. Fixing cross-age correlations at unity represents the null hypothesis assuming that females in different age classes share identical genetic background and thus we predict full genetic correlation between them (Lynch and Walsh 1998).

In addition to genetic effects, heterogeneous covariance matrices, with cross-age correlations fixed at unity, were fitted to the nest-of-rearing and nest-of-origin effects. These models were compared with a simpler model with heterogeneous variances in the additive genetic effect to make sure that brood and early parental effects (decomposed into rearing and origin nest effects by cross-fostering) do not inflate/bias our estimates of V¬A¬.

For all models, narrow-sense heritabilities (h2) of traits were calculated. To calculate h2 we divided respective VA by the sum of all variance components (Lynch and Walsh 1998). Standard errors of heritabilities were estimated using the delta-method (Lynch & Walsh 1998). Final models did not support genetic correlations between maternal age groups significantly lower than unity and thus we do not provide estimates of genetic correlations.

Technical notes

Our estimates of genetic variance might be biased as offspring from one nest-of-origin might not be full siblings. In our population, about 20% of nests contain extra-pair young (usually one nestling per nest; unpublished data from years not included in this study), resulting in overall prevalence of extra-pair young of approx. 4%. Such a level of extra-pair paternity should not strongly bias estimates of genetic variance, as predicted from simulation models (Charmantier and Reale 2005). Recent meta-analysis also suggests that bias in quantitative genetic studies introduced by errors in the pedigree may be less substantial than previously expected (Postma 2014). We have performed similar simulations, assuming the level of pedigree uncertainty similar to this observed in our population; these simulations indicate that small inconsistencies in the pedigree do not affect significantly even more complex estimated (co)variance structures (bias in differences in heritabilities and genetic correlations do not exceed 5%). Moreover, biases we have observed in simulated data affect observed differences in heritabilities downwardly and hence act conservatively. Finally, distribution of extra pair young shows no association with female age classes (χ2(df = 1) = 1.11, P = 0.29, based on data from the same population, years 2009-2011) and thus it is not likely to lead to the observed effect of maternal age. Other sources of pedigree error (such as intra-species brood parasitism) are not observed in our population.

Paternal age might contribute to the observed patterns if males and females in the studied population mate assortatively with respect to individual’s age. In such a case effects of paternal age might be inseparable from the effects of maternal age. However, this should not be the case in our population. Based on the available complete (i.e. both parents known) data on breeding pairs in the population in years 2004-2006 there is no evidence for assortative mating according to age (194 unique breeding pairs, test for assortativity according to age (two age classes): χ2(df = 1) = 0.33, P = 0.57). Moreover, experimental groups were formed with respect to maternal age as females can be more easily caught (on incubation) prior to hatching.

Results

Maternal age did not have any significant effect on any of the traits analyzed (body weight: P = 0.61, tarsus length: P = 0.58, PHA response: P = 0.74; Table 1, Appendix Table 1) but was retained in the model as it was used to structure covariance matrices for genetic and residual variance. Experimental brood manipulation affected all traits (Appendix Tables 1 and 2): offspring in experimentally increased broods were lighter (P < 0.001) and had shorter tarsi (P = 0.06). There was a trend of higher response to PHA in enlarged broods but it was not significant (P = 0.12). Models that attempted to split sources of phenotypic variation between maternal age groups and experimental groups (a 4×4 covariance matrix) had problems reaching convergence, which likely resulted from complex nature of fitted models. We therefore do not further discuss experimental manipulation in terms of partitioning of variance components. Experimental manipulation was not confounded with maternal age groups (Figure 1) and thus it cannot bias conclusions related to age – however, in all models considering age-specific effects on variance components experimental treatment is included as a fixed explanatory variable.

All random effects (except for quartet for tarsus length and additive genetic effect in body mass) appeared significant based on the likelihood-ratio test. Particularly, in tarsus length and PHA response we found a significant additive genetic component (Table 2).

Age-specific genetic variances were observed in PHA (Tables 2 and 3). VA in this trait appeared lower among old mothers’ offspring compared to young mothers’ offspring in case of (Table 3, Figure 2). Age specific residual variances in this trait were not supported (all model comparisons: P > 0.1). There was also no evidence for age specific variance related to nest-of-rearing (P = 0.09) and nest-of-origin (P = 0.99), indicating that permanent environmental effects do not depend on maternal age. Overall trait variances closely matched results from animal models (Table 1): total variance in PHA response was lower in offspring of old mothers.

VA differences translated directly into heritability differences. Heritability of PHA response was higher among offspring of young (heritability±SE: h2 = 0.68±0.09, Figure 2) compared to old mother’s offspring (h2 = 0.33±0.18, Figure 2). In tarsus length there were no maternal age-dependent differences in heritability (h2 = 0.38±0.08). In body mass heritability was non-significant (h2 = 0.29±0.12).

Discussion

We found a significant additive genetic variance in tarsus length and PHA response. After correcting for body size, we found also evidence for genetic variance in body mass, however it appeared non-significant. Our estimates of narrow sense heritabilities are similar to estimates reported elsewhere (Cichoń et al. 2006; Kruuk et al. 2001; Merilä and Fry 1998). More importantly, we demonstrated that maternal age is an important factor contributing to the complex picture of genotypic interactions. We found that additive genetic variance of immune response to PHA among offspring mothered by old females was substantially lower compared to the offspring of young females. In contrast, we found no cross-maternal-age differences in VA with respect to tarsus length.

Although evidence for interaction of parental age and genetics effects is scarce, genetic variances and genetic correlations depending on parental age have been already reported in laboratory populations of fruit flies and guppies (Beardmore et al. 1975; Beardmore and Shami 1985). Those studies demonstrated higher genetic variances among offspring of older parents and argued that this might represent ageing processes, in line with the predictions of the mutation accumulation hypothesis (Charmantier et al. 2014; Medawar 1952). However, interpretation of their results is difficult due to the fact that parental age is confounded with grand-parental age in their experimental design. Also, the controlled laboratory rearing conditions may not be appropriate to study age-specific heritabilities (Charmantier et al. 2014). Selective forces resulting from environmental heterogeneity certainly shape the genetic structure of natural populations, while in the laboratory populations selection is usually relaxed and does not reflect conditions under which a species had evolved. To our knowledge, only one study reported parental age to affect genetic variance in a wild bird population (Kim et al. 2011). It showed a significant decrease in genetic variance of age at first reproduction with respect to paternal age, but failed to detect any effects of maternal age. In this context, our study is complementary to Kim et al. (2011). However, our analyses rely on experimental data in which variation in parental age was a priori experimentally manipulated by matching broods of young and old females and cross-fostering nestlings to account for any confounding effects of rearing environment. It is an important advantage as environmental and early post-hatching parental effects might potentially be confounded with parental age if environmental effects are not randomly distributed across age classes leading to biased estimates of genetic variances. Cross-fostering also enables us to exclude early post-natal and maternal effects as likely drivers of observed differences: all such effects would inflate estimates of nest-of-origin variance which in our study remained consistently low, and homogenous between maternal age groups.

Our results add to the growing evidence that the genetic architecture of wild populations is complex (Jensen et al. 2003; Nystrand et al. 2011; Poissant et al. 2010; Seppala and Jokela 2010; Wilson et al. 2007), and demonstrates the potential importance of considering parental age in quantitative genetic studies, especially when applying analyses based on full-sib comparisons without employing animal model. The estimates of VA presented here clearly suggest that younger mothers may contribute more to the overall genetic variation observed in the population in specific traits. As a consequence, the response to selection may be stronger among the offspring of younger mothers. In many animal taxa clear age-structuring is observed in populations (e.g. Laws et al. 2010; Pelletier et al. 2012; Soulsbury et al. 2011). Thus, any changes in age-structure of a reproducing population may alter its evolutionary trajectory and effective response to selection in cohorts expressing more genetic variance would dominate overall evolutionary change in population. More specific predictions of evolutionary dynamics of such systems are likely to be challenging as both offspring phenotypes and parental traits (age) are taken into account. Univariate breeder’s equation may be inappropriate in such situations (McAdam et al. 2014) which motivates further detailed simulation studies.

An intriguing question on the origin of the observed patterns of age-specific genetic variances arises. Offspring of older parents may become more genetically uniform if specific genotypes are selectively removed from the population across age classes. In our population, survival rate from one to two years of age does not exceed 40% (Podmokła et al., in preparation). Thus, there is potential for selection to operate. Immune function have repeatedly been shown to predict subsequent survival and reproductive success (Alatalo and Lundberg 1986; Møller and Saino 2004; Norris and Evans 2000). In particular, data gathered in the studied population support the presence of significant selection acting on immunocompetence measured by PHA response (Cichoń and Dubiec 2005). Unfortunately data gathered in the current study do not allow for direct estimation of selection gradients and thus selection can be treated only as one of possible mechanisms. Moreover, direct selection (suggested by Cichoń and Dubiec 2005) does not seem to be supported by our data as we have observed no significant difference in the mean PHA response in offspring between young vs. old mothers. Certainly, this doesn’t rule out stabilizing selection, however more in-depth genetic analyses are required to support selective explanation.

Even in the absence of any selection acting on a trait, it’s quantitative genetics may be substantially altered by environmental conditions experienced by individuals. Numerous studies have provided evidence, that environment may influence levels of observed genetic variance (Gienapp and Brommer 2014; Hallsson and Bjorklund 2012; Hoffmann and Merila 1999; Ingleby et al. 2010; King et al. 2011; Nystrand et al. 2011; Wolinska and King 2009). Such mechanism would be possible in the presented case as often first-time breeders are less successful in caring for their young and securing high-quality habitats for them (Angelier 2006) and thus would provide them with markedly different rearing environment. Such environmental heterogeneity might generate genotype-by-environment interactions (G×E) and result in the observed age-specific patterns of VA, particularly because immunocompetence strongly depends on multiple aspects of parental care (Ilmonen et al. 2003; Moreno et al. 1999; Saino et al. 1997). Another source of maternal age-related variation in environments experienced by nestlings include varying success of young and old females in securing high quality males. Older females – as more experienced – often tend to be more choosy or base their mate choice on different male characteristics than younger ones (Candolin 2003; Jouventin et al. 1999). This process alone could provide offspring with different “paternal” rearing environments, triggering interactions of genetic effects with varying experienced conditions. In either of these explanations female age has to be associated with some environmental features or father’s traits which points to studies exploring such associations as valuable extensions of our findings.

The abovementioned explanations are potentially universal in terms of affected traits, but our study may be specifically limited because of the choice of only one immunocompetence metric we have measured. Immune function is a complex multidimensional trait, thus conclusions drawn from a single measure of immune response to an artificial antigen should be taken with caution. In addition, the use of hypersensitivity reaction to PHA as a measure general immune function has been criticized by some authors (Demas et al. 2011; Sarv and Horak 2009; Vinkler and Albrecht 2011; Vinkler et al. 2012). However, the PHA test is commonly used and a number of studies reported significant heritability of reaction to PHA and more importantly is shows a significant correlation with a number of traits considered to be a fitness proxies, such as survival and recruitment (Cichoń and Dubiec 2005; Møller and Saino 2004). Here we provide analyses that lead to an important insight into the understanding of complex structure of quantitative traits and do not aim at generalizing our result concerning immune response to PHA as being representative for a proxy of immune function as a whole.

To conclude, our study provides the first experimental evidence that additive genetic variance observed among offspring depends on maternal age. In age-structured populations such genetic heterogeneity may result in different age classes contributing differently to the overall genetic variation of the population and age-specificity of response to selection.

Funding and Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Marta Cholewa, Dariusz Wiejaczka and Katarzyna Kulma for assistance with the fieldwork. We also would like to thank Simon Evans, Loeske Kruuk and several anonymous referees for constructive comments on the manuscript. Financial support was provided by the State Committee for Scientific Research of Poland in 2004–2007 (to AD), by the Jagiellonian University (within the project Society – Environment – Technology co-financed by the European Union) and by the Foundation for Polish Science (to SMD). The long term nest box study was supported by The Swedish Research Council (Grant to LG). The study conforms to the legal requirements of Sweden. All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Authors:

Szymon M. Drobniak, Institute of Environmental Sciences, Jagiellonian University, ul. Gronostajowa 7, 30-387 Kraków, Poland; tel. +48 12 664 51 79; e-mail: szymek.drobniak@uj.edu.pl

Anna Dubiec, Museum and Institute of Zoology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warszawa, Poland

Lars Gustafsson, Departament of Ecology and Genetics/Animal Ecology, Evolutionary Biology Center, Uppsala University, Sweden

Mariusz Cichoń, Institute of Environmental Sciences, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland

Corresponding author: Szymon Drobniak, Institute of Environmental Sciences, Jagiellonian University, ul. Gronostajowa 7, 30-387 Kraków, Poland; tel. +48 12 664 51 79; e-mail: szymek.drobniak@uj.edu.pl

Abstract

Studies examining age-specific patterns in genetic variance have focussed primarily on changes in genetic variance within cohorts. It remains unclear whether parental age may affect the genetic variance among offspring. To date, such an effect has been reported only in a single study performed in a wild bird population. Here, we provide experimental evidence that the additive genetic variance (VA) observed among offspring may be related to parental age in a wild passerine – the blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus). To separate genetic and environmental components of phenotypic variance in nestling body size and immune function we cross-fostered nestlings between pairs of broods born to young and old mothers and used an animal model to estimate VA. We show that the genetic variance in immune response to phytohaemaglutinin (PHA) and body weight among offspring depends on maternal age. VA in response to PHA appeared to be lower among nestlings of older mothers. Such a tendency was not observed for tarsus length. We argue that the lower VA may result either from depletion of additive genetic variation due to selection acting on parents across age classes or from environmental effects confounded with parental age. Thus, our study suggests that parental age may significantly affect estimates of quantitative genetic variance.

 

References

Alatalo RV, Lundberg A (1986) Heritability and Selection on Tarsus Length in the Pied Flycatcher (Ficedula-Hypoleuca). Evolution 40:574-583

Alonso-Alvarez C, Tella JL (2001) Effects of experimental food restriction and body-mass changes on the avian T-cell-mediated immune response. Canadian Journal of Zoology 79:101-105

Angelier F, Weimerskirch, H., Dano, S., Chastel, O. (2006) Age, experience and reproductive performance in a long-lived bird: a hormonal perspective. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61:611-621

Beardmore JA, Lints F, Albaldawi ALF (1975) Parental Age and Heritability of Sternopleural Chaeta Number in Drosophila-Melanogaster. Heredity 34:71-82

Beardmore JA, Shami SA (1985) The Lansing Effect and Age-Mediated Changes in Genetic-Parameters. Genetica 68:37-46

Blondel J, Perret P, Anstett MC, Thebaud C (2002) Evolution of sexual size dimorphism in birds: test of hypotheses using blue tits in contrasted Mediterranean habitats. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 15:440-450

Bouwhuis S, Charmantier A, Verhulst S, Sheldon BC (2010) Trans-generational effects on ageing in a wild bird population. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 23:636-642

Brommer JE, Rattiste K, Wilson A (2010) The rate of ageing in a long-lived bird is not heritable. Heredity 104:363-370

Butler D (2009) asreml: asreml() fits the linear mixed model. R-package version 3.0-1. VSNi

Candolin U (2003) The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biological Reviews 78:575-595

Charlesworth B, Hughes, K. A. (2000) The maintenance of genetic variaition in life-history traits. Evolutionary genetics: From molecules to morphology 1:369-392

Charmantier A, Brommer JE, Nussey DH (2014) The quantitative genetics of senescence in wild animals. In: Charmantier A, Garant, D., Kruuk, L. E. B. (ed) Quantitative genetics in the wild. Oxford University Press, Oxford, United Kingdom, pp 68-83

Charmantier A, Reale D (2005) How do misassigned paternities affect the estimation of heritability in the wild? Molecular Ecology 14:2839-2850

Cichoń M, Dubiec A (2005) Cell-mediated immunity predicts the probability of local recruitment in nestling blue tits. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 18:962-966

Cichoń M, Sendecka J, Gustafsson L (2006) Genetic and environmental variation in immune response of collared flycatcher nestlings. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 19:1701-1706

Demas GE, Zysling DA, Beechler BR, Muehlenbein MP, French SS (2011) Beyond phytohaemagglutinin: assessing vertebrate immune function across ecological contexts. Journal of Animal Ecology 80:710-730

Drobniak SM, Wiejaczka D, Arct A, Dubiec A, Gustafsson L, Cichon M (2010) Sex-specific heritability of cell-mediated immune response in the blue tit nestlings (Cyanistes caeruleus). Journal of Evolutionary Biology 23:1286-1292

Garnett MC (1981) Body Size, Its Heritability and Influence on Juvenile Survival among Great Tits, Parus-Major. Ibis 123:31-41

Gienapp P, Brommer JE (2014) Evolutionary dynamics in response to climate change. In: Charmantier A, Garant, D., Kruuk, L. E. B. (ed) Quantitative genetics in the wild. Oxford University Press, Oxford, United Kingdom, pp 254-270

Goto N, Kodama H, Okada K, Fujimoto Y (1978) Suppression of Phytohemagglutinin Skin-Response in Thymectomized Chickens. Poultry Science 57:246-250

Hall MD, Lailvaux SP, Blows MW, Brooks RC (2010) Sexual Conflict and the Maintenance of Multivariate Genetic Variation. Evolution 64:1697-1703

Hallsson LR, Bjorklund M (2012) Sex-specific genetic variances in life-history and morphological traits of the seed beetle Callosobruchus maculatus. Ecology and Evolution 2:128-138

Henderson CR (1950) Estimation of Genetic Parameters. Annals of Mathematical Statistics 21:309-310

Henderson CR (1984) Applications of linear models in animal breeding. University of Guelph Press, Guelph, Canada

Hoffmann AA, Merila J (1999) Heritable variation and evolution under favourable and unfavourable conditions. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 14:96-101

Ilmonen P, Hasselquist D, Langefors A, Wiehn J (2003) Stress, immunocompetence and leukocyte profiles of pied flycatchers in relation to brood size manipulation. Oecologia 136:148-154

Ingleby FC, Hunt J, Hosken DJ (2010) The role of genotype-by-environment interactions in sexual selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 23:2031-2045

Jensen H, Saether BE, Ringsby TH, Tufto J, Griffith SC, Ellegren H (2003) Sexual variation in heritability and genetic correlations of morphological traits in house sparrow (Passer domesticus). Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16:1296-1307

Jouventin P, Lequette B, Dobson FS (1999) Age-related mate choice in the wandering albatross. Animal Behaviour 57:1099-1106

Kilpimaa J, Van de Casteele T, Jokinen I, Mappes J, Alatalo RV (2005) Genetic and environmental variation in antibody and T-cell mediated responses in the great tit. Evolution 59:2483-2489

Kim SY, Drummond H, Torres R, Velando A (2011) Evolvability of an avian life history trait declines with father’s age. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 24:295-302

King MO, Owen JP, Schwabl H (2011) Injecting the Mite into Ecological Immunology: Measuring the Antibody Response of House Sparrows (Passer domesticus) Challenged with Hematophagous Mites. Auk 128:340-345

Kruuk LEB (2004) Estimating genetic parameters in natural populations using the ‘animal model’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 359:873-890

Kruuk LEB, Hadfield JD (2007) How to separate genetic and environmental causes of similarity between relatives. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20:1890-1903

Kruuk LEB, Merila J, Sheldon BC (2001) Phenotypic selection on a heritable size trait revisited. American Naturalist 158:557-571

Lande R, Shannon S (1996) The role of genetic variation in adaptation and population persistence in a changing environment. Evolution 50:434-437

Laws RJ, Townsend SM, Nakagawa S, Jamieson IG (2010) Limited inbreeding depression in a bottlenecked population is age but not environment dependent. Journal of Avian Biology 41:645-652

Liu YS, Zhi MX, Li XJ (2011) Parental age and characteristics of the offspring. Ageing Research Reviews 10:115-123

Lynch M, Walsh B (1998) Genetics and analysis of quantitative traits. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA, USA

McAdam AG, Garant D, Wilson AJ (2014) The effects of others’ genes: metrnal and other indirect genetic effects. In: Charmantier A, Garant, D., Kruuk, L. E. B. (ed) Quantitative genetics in the wild. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 84-103

Medawar PB (1952) An unsolved problem of biology. H.K. Lewis & Co., London, United Kingdom

Merilä J, Fry JD (1998) Genetic variation and causes of genotype-environment interaction in the body size of blue tit (Parus caeruleus). Genetics 148:1233-1244

Møller AP, Saino N (2004) Immune response and survival. Oikos 104:299-304

Moore PJ, Harris WE (2003) Is a decline in offspring quality a necessary consequence of maternal age? Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 270:S192-S194

Moreno J, Sanz JJ, Arriero E (1999) Reproductive effort and T-lymphocyte cell-mediated immunocompetence in female pied flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 266:1105-1109

Morrissey MB, Wilson AJ (2010) pedantics: an r package for pedigree-based genetic simulation and pedigree manipulation, characterization and viewing. Molecular Ecology Resources 10:711-719

Norris K, Evans MR (2000) Ecological immunology: life history trade-offs and immune defense in birds. Behavioral Ecology 11:19-26

Nystrand M, Dowling DK, Simmons LW (2011) Complex Genotype by Environment interactions and changing genetic architectures across thermal environments in the Australian field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus. BMC Evolutionary Biology 11

Pärt T, Gustafsson L (1989) Breeding Dispersal in the Collared Flycatcher (Ficedula-Albicollis) – Possible Causes and Reproductive Consequences. Journal of Animal Ecology 58:305-320

Pelletier F, Moyes K, Clutton-Brock TH, Coulson T (2012) Decomposing variation in population growth into contributions from environment and phenotypes in an age-structured population. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 279:394-401

Pinheiro JC, Bates DM (2000) Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. Springer, New York, NY, USA

Pitala N, Siitari H, Gustafsson L, Brommer JE (2009) Ectoparasites help to maintain variation in cell-mediated immunity in the blue tit-hen flea system. Evolutionary Ecology Research 11:79-94

Poissant J, Wilson AJ, Coltman DW (2010) Sex-Specific Genetic Variance and the Evolution of Sexual Dimorphism: A Systematic Review of Cross-Sex Genetic Correlations. Evolution 64:97-107

Postma E (2014) Four decades of estimating heritabilities in wild vertebrate populations: improved methods, more data, better estimates? In: Charmantier A, Garant, D., Kruuk, L. E. B. (ed) Quantitative genetics in the wild. Oxford University Press, Oxford, United Kingdom, pp 16-33

Priest NK, Mackowiak B, Promislow DEL (2002) The role of parental age effects on the evolution of aging. Evolution 56:927-935

R Core Team (2014) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria

Roff DA (1997) Evolutionary quantitative genetics. Chapman and Hall, New York, NY, USA

Saino N, Calza S, Moller AP (1997) Immunocompetence of nestling barn swallows in relation to brood size and parental effort. Journal of Animal Ecology 66:827-836

Sarv T, Horak P (2009) Phytohaemagglutinin injection has a long-lasting effect on immune cells. Journal of Avian Biology 40:569-571

Self SG, Liang KY (1987) Asymptotic Properties of Maximum-Likelihood Estimators and Likelihood Ratio Tests under Nonstandard Conditions. Journal of the American Statistical Association 82:605-610

Seppala O, Jokela J (2010) Maintenance of Genetic Variation in Immune Defense of a Freshwater Snail: Role of Environmental Heterogeneity. Evolution 64:2397-2407

Soulsbury CD, Alatalo RV, Lebigre C, Rokka K, Siitari H (2011) Age-dependent inbreeding risk and offspring fitness costs in female black grouse. Biology Letters 7:853-855

Storfer A (1996) Quantitative genetics: A promising approach for the assessment of genetic variation in endangered species. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 11:343-348

Vinkler M, Albrecht T (2011) Handling immunocompetence’ in ecological studies: do we operate with confused terms? Journal of Avian Biology 42:490-493

Vinkler M, Schnitzer J, Munclinger P, Albrecht T (2012) Phytohaemagglutinin skin-swelling test in scarlet rosefinch males: low-quality birds respond more strongly. Animal Behaviour 83:17-23

Weiss LA, Pan L, Abney M, Ober C (2006) The sex-specific genetic architecture of quantitative traits in humans. Nature Genetics 38:218-222

Wilson AJ et al. (2007) Evidence for a genetic basis of aging in two wild vertebrate populations. Current Biology 17:2136-2142

Wolinska J, King KC (2009) Environment can alter selection in host-parasite interactions. Trends in Parasitology 25:236-244

Tables

Table 1. Means and variances of all analyzed traits, split between young and old genetic mothers.

Trait Genetic mother Mean Variance CV
Tarsus length [mm] Young 16.17 0.42 0.12
Old 16.16 0.43 0.12
Body weight [g] Young 10.59 1.01 0.12
Old 10.62 1.05 0.12
PHA response [mm] Young 0.82 0.10 0.40
Old 0.74 0.04 0.26

Table 3. Likelihood-ratio tests of variance components. Head labels: log(L) – logarithm of likelihood; Δlog(L) – difference in log-likelihoods of the more complex and simpler model; P – significance of the random effect added in the more complex model, as compared to the simpler model; Test – indicates which models were compared. The last column provides the interpretation of each model comparison.

Modela No. Test log(L) Δlog(L) P Significance of…
Tarsus length
E 1 -56.21
E Q 2 2 vs. 1 -56.21 0 experimental quartet effect
E R 3 3 vs. 1 45.32 101.52 <0.001 nest-of-rearing effect
E R O 4 4 vs. 3 69.67 24.35 <0.001 nest-of-origin effect
E R O A 5 5 vs. 4 72.55 2.88 0.008 additive genetic effect (VA)
E R O Age(A) 6 6 vs. 5 73.14 0.58 0.146 age dependence of VA
Age(E) R O A 7 7 vs. 5 73.34 0.79 0.103 age dependence of residual variance (VE)
Age(E) R O Age(A) 8 8 vs. 7 73.35 0.01 0.499 test for confounding effect of VE on VA
8 vs. 6 73.35 0.78 0.103
Body mass
E 1 -302.24
E Q 2 2 vs. 1 -300.09 2.14 0.038 experimental quartet effect
E Q R 3 3 vs. 2 -156.61 143.47 <0.001 nest-of-rearing effect
E Q R O 4 4 vs. 3 -134.18 22.43 <0.001 nest-of-origin effect
E Q R O A 5 5 vs. 4 -133.27 0.91 0.061 additive genetic effect (VA)
PHA response
E 1 360.21
E Q 2 2 vs. 1 367.87 7.61 <0.001 experimental quartet effect
E Q R 3 3 vs. 2 415.13 47.3 <0.001 nest-of-rearing effect (VR)
E Q R O 4 4 vs. 3 430.06 14.94 <0.001 nest-of-origin effect (VO)
E Q R O A 5 5 vs. 4 431.64 1.57 0.003 additive genetic effect (VA)
E Q R O Age(A) 6 6 vs. 5 447.96 16.33 <0.001 age dependence of VA
Age(E) Q R O A 7 7 vs. 5 431.65 0.01 0.499 age dependence of residual variance (VE)
Age(E) Q R O Age(A) 8 8 vs. 6 448 0.04 0.479 test for confounding effect of VE on VA
8 vs. 7 448 16.35 <0.001 age dependence of VA in presence of age-dependent VE
E Q R O Age(A)b 9 9 vs. 6 448.02 0.06 0.485 cross-age genetic covariance lower than unity
E Q Age(R) Age(O) Age(A) 10 10 vs. 6 449.09 2.77 0.09 test for confounding effect of VR and VO on VA
E Q Age(R) O Age(A) 11 11 vs. 6 449.08 2.76 0.09 test for confounding effect of VR on VA
E Q R Age(O) Age(A) 12 12 vs. 6 447.95 0.01 0.99 test for confounding effect of VO on VA

aTerms in models are labeled in the following way: E – residual variance; Q – quartet; R – nest of rearing; O – nest of origin; A – additive genetic effect; Age(X) – indicates that a (constrained) age-dependent covariance matrix is fitted (cross-age correlations constrained to unity for A and zero for E).

bResulting covariance matrix is unconstrained (covariance is estimated).

Materials on similar topics